Aging stereotypes--internalization or inoculation? A commentary.

نویسنده

  • Leslie A Zebrowitz
چکیده

I was 7 years old when I can first remember my ‘‘Bubbie’’ getting ready for bed during an overnight visit. With some curiosity, I watched her put her teeth into a glass of water and noticed the sagging flesh on her upper arms as she donned her nightgown, and I knew, without being told, that these physical peculiarities were associated with age. At that moment, I resolved to take good care of my teeth, and later, when I knew something about fitness, I exercised my triceps to prevent the sagging flesh. These reactions to my beliefs about the physical effects of aging are probably not an anomaly. Indeed, there is good reason to believe that aging individuals are more resistant to the internalization of beliefs about aging than they are given credit for in Levy’s (2003) description of the development of aging self-stereotypes. Perhaps the most important factor fostering resistance to the internalization of aging stereotypes is a tendency for aging adults to deny their membership in the category ‘‘elderly.’’ Older adults report feeling younger than their chronological age, and they perceive their own developmental goals to be more consistent with the goals of adults younger than themselves (Heckhausen, 1997; Montepare & Lachman, 1989). Older adults also resist the category label ‘‘elderly,’’ preferring to call themselves ‘‘senior citizens,’’ ‘‘retired persons,’’ or ‘‘mature adults’’ (Montepare & Lachman, 1989; Ward, 1984). These explicit effects on self-categorization are mirrored by implicit measures of age identity that show that older individuals identify with the category ‘‘young’’ as strongly as do young individuals (Levy and Banaji, 2002). In essence, many older adults do not identify with the stigmatized group, an achievement that is quite feasible given the blurry demarcation of age group membership. Although Levy (2003) suggests that the ingroup preference does not apply among the old, the fact is that older adults may define their ingroup differently from the category into which younger adults place them. Aging adults’ eschewal of the category elderly may be related to individuating processes in impression formation (Brewer, 1988; Fiske & Neuberg, 1989). Research has shown that people are less likely to attribute category stereotypes to others when they have information about their unique attributes or when they are motivated to seek such information, an effect that has been documented in judgments of aging adults (e.g., Fiske, Neuberg, Beattie, & Milberg, 1987; Kite & Johnson, 1988; Neuberg & Fiske, 1987). Judgments about the self are made with abundant individuating information and high motivation that should work against the application of category stereotypes. Indeed, it may be far easier for people to resist internalizing aging stereotypes than racial or gender stereotypes, inasmuch as they can draw on a lifetime of individuating information that contradicts aging stereotypes. Moreover, because aging adults do not identify with the category elderly, the negative attitudes that young children express toward getting older (cf. Montepare & Zebrowitz, 2002) may ultimately be applied to themselves more rarely than Levy (2003) expects. A life-long belief that aging stereotypes apply only to others argues against Levy’s hypothesis that uncritical acceptance of age stereotypes when they are initially encountered later produces strong effects on the self. The relatively youthful self-perceptions of many aging adults may inoculate them against behavioral confirmation of aging stereotypes. Indeed, people who are aware that negative stereotypes may be applied to them often compensate rather than capitulate, and self-fulfilling prophecies are not so readily obtained as some have assumed (cf. Snyder, 1992). As illustrated in the anecdote about my reactions to my aging Bubbie, people may take actions to ward off undesirable possible selves (Cross & Markus, 1991; Markus & Nurius, 1986). Behavioral confirmation of aging stereotypes also may fail to occur (a) because people engage in self-verification, behaving in a manner that provides corrective feedback to others with erroneous trait expectations (e.g., Swann & Ely, 1984); (b) because people show compensatory behavior that is opposite to disagreeable expectations (e.g., Miller & Myers, 1998; Zebrowitz, Andreoletti, Collins, Lee, & Blumenthal, 1998); or even (c) because those holding the stereotypes show compensatory behavior, behaving in ways that elicit behaviors opposite to those they expect (e.g., Bond, 1972). If aging adults do not identify with the category elderly and are inoculated against internalizing aging stereotypes, then how can researchers account for the negative effects of these stereotypes on older adults’ cognitive and physical functioning that Levy (2003) has documented? For one thing, the behavioral effects of stereotype activation do not require self-stereotyping. Indeed, activating elderly stereotypes can also affect the behavior of young individuals (e.g., Bargh, Chen, & Burrows, 1996; Dijksterhuis, Aarts, Bargh, & Van Knippenberg, 2000; see Wheeler & Petty, 2001, for a review of the behavioral effects of activating otherand self-stereotypes). Although Levy (2003) reports effects of elderly stereotype activation on older but not younger adults, this need not be attributed to differential internalization of elderly stereotypes. Indeed, there are other variables that can moderate the effects of stereotype activation manipulations. Greater ‘‘age-schematicity’’ in older adults (Montepare & Clements, 2001) may increase their susceptibility to the activation of elderly stereotypes without necessarily implying greater internalization. ‘‘Stereotype-threat’’ may make older adults more behaviorally responsive to the activation of elderly stereotypes even if they do not internalize them (cf. Steele, 1997; Steele & Aronson, 1995). Since ‘‘cognitive load’’ may facilitate the application of an activated stereotype (Gilbert & Hixon, 1991), older adults may show a greater effect of any stereotype activation if their cognitive load is higher when performing various experimental tasks. Similarly, weakened ‘‘inhibitory processes’’ in older adults may contribute to greater behavioral effects of activated stereotypes (von Hipple, Silver, & Lynch, 2000). To determine why the activation of aging stereotypes has a greater effect on the behavior of older than younger adults will require additional research to identify the underlying mechanism, which may include one or more of the possibilities discussed above. Such research should include a comparison of older and younger adults’ behavioral responses to the activation of stereotypes other than elderly ones. Without such research it is premature to attribute effects of primed age COMMENTARIES AND AUTHOR’S REPLY P214

برای دانلود متن کامل این مقاله و بیش از 32 میلیون مقاله دیگر ابتدا ثبت نام کنید

ثبت نام

اگر عضو سایت هستید لطفا وارد حساب کاربری خود شوید

منابع مشابه

Subjective Health: The Roles of Communication, Language, Aging, Stereotypes, and Culture

A consensually-agreed position among scholars of communication and aging is that while psychological and physical health mutually impact each other, the quality of language to and from older adult individuals shape each of these—and are shaped by them. Encounters with others inside and outside of one’s age ingroup involve stereotyped expectations with regard to language and other speec...

متن کامل

Elderly and Social Ethics

Background: Negative beliefs and stereotypes about aging guide the lifestyle of people in a society and its behavioral and psychological consequences affect mental and physical health. Many individual and social problems have moral roots. In the shadow of social ethics, the social security of individuals is maintained. The present study examines the attitude of society towards aging and immoral...

متن کامل

Assessing aging stereotypes: Personal stereotypes, self-stereotypes and self-perception of aging.

BACKGROUND There is a broad semantic network of aging stereotypes; where different concepts and their measurement are confused: personal stereotypes, self-stereotypes and self-perception of aging. METHOD First, we analyze the translated version of the Image of Aging Scale (IAS) measurement model through exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis, with two representative sub-samples of the S...

متن کامل

Internalization of weight bias: Implications for binge eating and emotional well-being.

OBJECTIVE This study examined the relationship between internalization of negative weight-based stereotypes and indices of eating behaviors and emotional well-being in a sample of overweight and obese women. RESEARCH METHOD AND PROCEDURES The sample was comprised of 1013 women who belonged to a national, non-profit weight loss organization. Participants completed an on-line battery of self-re...

متن کامل

Lifespan Development: A Social-Cultural Perspective

This thesis explores some of the social factors that may affect individuals as they age. A lifespan developmental perspective is employed in investigating the effects of societal aging stereotypes on will-to-live and risk-taking skills. Results suggest negative aging stereotypes may have deleterious effects on the elderly, but not young individuals in terms of will-to-live, but have no effect o...

متن کامل

ذخیره در منابع من


  با ذخیره ی این منبع در منابع من، دسترسی به آن را برای استفاده های بعدی آسان تر کنید

برای دانلود متن کامل این مقاله و بیش از 32 میلیون مقاله دیگر ابتدا ثبت نام کنید

ثبت نام

اگر عضو سایت هستید لطفا وارد حساب کاربری خود شوید

عنوان ژورنال:
  • The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences

دوره 58 4  شماره 

صفحات  -

تاریخ انتشار 2003